Bob,
Thanks for your kind words and your patience. Please forgive me if I misinterpreted any of your statements, I really try to be accurate about such things. And be assured that my intention is only to contribute to the discussion, not pull it off track in any way.
I realize this is not a discussion about ambiance or soundstage enhancement, that is simply the example with which I am best able to illustrate my point. Thank you for humoring me, I know both you and John are extremely busy guys.
Dear AudioCRaver: What is the distance of the Martin Logans from the front wall? How is the front wall treated?
From electrostatic panel to front wall is 56 inches, a little under 5 feet. The reflected energy reaches the LP after a delay of 8.2 ms. The front wall is finished drywall with slightly textured paint. The speakers are 63 inches apart center-to-center. The reflective panels are flat, unpainted boards, each 12 inches wide, spaced 80 inches apart center-to-center and the entire area of the wall between the panels is treated with absorptive material.
As far as I can see, your use of reflections to help create a soundstage has little to do the likelihood that your perception of the direct sound from the loudspeakers is the primary high frequency response.
While we're at it, I'd like to reference a U.S. Patent on my own invention that uses a specific set of delays to enhance ambience, spatiality and soundstage. Somewhere around or under 30 ms is the magic time for that, but the devil is in the details. I just wanted to mention that because since you mentioned Haas I thought I would mention that Haas and I are very good friends

.
Very cool, I will look it up!
I doubt that your ear/brain is wired any differently from other humans, so it's highly largely your perception of the frequency response of your loudspeakers is within less than 10 ms. and the rest of the effects you hear that enhance the soundstage are independent of your judgment of the high frequency response.
I was referring only to the ability to "learn to listen," or focus, differently, based on interest, need, exposure, etc. The non-sighted person will "hear" a room differently from you or I out of the intense desire to not be tripped or run into a wall or fall out of a window. Your decades of experience in the studio and mastering room have no doubt taught you to hear a lot in a room and speakers that others would overlook. While a few psychoacoustical studies focus on musicians and/or experienced "critical listeners," most focus on the general population. As we learn to listen to finer and finer levels of detail, for whatever reason, I believe it possible, even probable, that some of the psychoacoustical limits defined by those important studies get stretched, sometimes quite a bit.
Anyway, never fear. The variable window should be user adjustable to account for a range of tastes or other circumstances.
Thank you, that answers my concern!
Also, John and I (and anyone else who would like to chime in) have to delve further into the mysteries of Acourate to figure out why its high frequency determination is so much brighter than REW's, when it appears NOT to be window width. Mysteries, mysteries, mysteries.
Indeed, ain't life fun!
Oh by the way, I did not exactly say that "we hear the speaker and not the room". What I said is that authorities such as Jim Johnston have researched that the ear's perception of a loudspeaker's response approaches anechoic at high frequencies, that at high frequencies the brain ignores even the earliest reflections. So a true high frequency measurement should begin to ignore reflections as the frequencies rise.
I do beg your pardon if I put words in your mouth. The phrase is used from time to time referring to all the frequencies in a room above the Schroeder frequency, and while that might not be too inaccurate for
general discussion, when referring to the
general listener, this is a discussion about picky details as perceived by highly experienced, sensitive listeners, and would be an oversimplification under the circumstances.
Repeating part of your above quote...
...a true high frequency measurement should begin to ignore reflections as the frequencies rise.
...(my added emphasis) I would agree is a more accurate way to state the phenomenon. And just HOW MUCH to ignore it is the perplexing mystery under investigation.
This certainly does not exclude such phenomena as soundstage or ambience enhancement by use of early reflections or bidirectional loudspeakers. In those cases the ear certainly does hear the room. Our perception of the frequency response of a loudspeaker system at lowest frequencies on up through the midrange includes much of the room response as well as the direct response. Hope this helps.
Thank you again for the detailed explanation. I am satisfied that the phenomenon is being treated as a tendency, the sensitivity of which is not fully understood.
I am close to your age, and my audio-centric roles have been numerous, some for pleasure and some professional, including a lot of hours staring at RTA plots and playing with parametric EQ bands just to see what they sound like and how small a change I can detect, and have gotten pretty good at sorting through the details of what the differences might be between sound A and sound B - not unique wiring, as you say, but ability to focus and discriminate as driven by an obsessive curiosity that borders on the ridiculous at times. Well aware that much of the soundstage enhancement effects take place in mid frequencies, the following measurements from my system might help illustrate my point.
All smoothing is 1/12th octave. All windowing referred to is Tukey 0.25 right window.
1. Here is the measurement at the LP of the signal from the front of the electrostatic panel only. The rear signal is being absorbed by thick towels draped over the back of the panel. Note that the plots for 5 ms window and 15 ms window virtually overlay each other above 1 kHz, showing that there is no contribution above 1 kHz from the absorbed rear, reflected signal, which would fall completely within the 15 ms gated window. It says "there is no reflected signal, it is being completely absorbed."
2. With the towels removed and the rear, reflected signal freely combining with the front signal after the 8.2 ms delay, with 5 ms gating. It is like saying "we would not hear the high frequencies above 3 kHz, so let's gate them out of the measurement." If that is true, according to these plots, removing the towels should result in no discernible change to the response above 3 kHz.
3. But I DO hear SOME brightening of the response IN THAT RANGE. The trained ear can focus on that area of frequency response detail, as I am sure you do every day. By opening the window for the combined plot (green), we can see there is quite a bit more information the MIGHT be heard by the TRAINED ear. How much, how high? I don't know (shoulder shrug).
4. As a reference, here is an impulse plot of the direct and reflected sounds.
My ears are not special, and neither are yours. Well, ok, you are Bob Katz, so maybe yours are a
little bit special. Laugh with me here. Like A.J., I like what you are doing, just trying to help make sure that one key assumption is being handled properly, and I am quite satisfied now that it is.
I have said enough. You have addressed my concern, and I thank you. If I see a way to contribute to the project, I will try to climb in without swamping the canoe. Otherwise I will follow along quietly, knowing the work to be in excellent hands. Thanks again for listening.